John Robert Anderson (psychologist)

Summary

John Robert Anderson (born August 27, 1947) is a Canadian-born American psychologist. He is currently professor of Psychology and Computer Science at Carnegie Mellon University.

John Robert Anderson
Born (1947-08-27) August 27, 1947 (age 76)
Alma materUniversity of British Columbia (B.A.)
Stanford University (Ph.D.)
Known forIntelligent tutoring systems
Cognitive tutors
ACT-R
Rational analysis
Scientific career
FieldsEducational psychology
Cognitive psychology (mathematics education)
InstitutionsCarnegie Mellon University
ThesisA stochastic model of sentence memory (1972)
Doctoral advisorGordon Bower
Notable studentsNeil Heffernan
Kenneth Koedinger
Christian Lebiere
Peter Pirolli
Dario Salvucci
Lael Schooler

Biography edit

Anderson obtained a B.A. from the University of British Columbia in 1968, and a Ph.D. in Psychology from Stanford in 1972. He became an assistant professor at Yale in 1972. He moved to the University of Michigan in 1973 as a Junior Fellow (and married Lynne Reder who was a graduate student there) and returned to Yale in 1976 with tenure. He was promoted to full professor at Yale in 1977 but moved to Carnegie Mellon University in 1978. From 1988 to 1989, he served as president of the Cognitive Science Society. He was elected to the American Academy of Arts and Sciences and the National Academy of Sciences and has received a series of awards:

  • 1968: Governor General's Gold Medal: Graduated as top student in Arts and Sciences at University of British Columbia
  • 1978: Early Career Award of the American Psychological Association
  • 1989–1994: Research Scientist Award, NIMH
  • 1994: American Psychological Association's Distinguished Scientific Career Award
  • 1999: Elected to the National Academy of Sciences[1]
  • 1999: Fellow of American Academy of Arts and Sciences[2]
  • 2004: The David E. Rumelhart Prize,[3] for Contributions to the Formal Analysis of Human Cognition
  • 2005: Howard Crosby Warren Medal for outstanding achievement in Experimental Psychology in the United States and Canada, Society of Experimental Psychology
  • 2006: Inaugural Dr. A.H. Heineken Prize[4] for Cognitive Science awarded by the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences
  • 2011: Benjamin Franklin Medal in Computer and Cognitive Science, Franklin Institute[5] "for the development of the first large-scale computational theory of the process by which humans perceive, learn and reason, and its application to computer tutoring systems."
  • 2016: Atkinson Prize from the National Academy of Sciences.

Research edit

In cognitive psychology, John Anderson is widely known for his cognitive architecture ACT-R[6][7] and rational analysis.[8][9] He has published many papers on cognitive psychology, including recent criticism of unjustified claims in mathematics education that lack experimental warrant and sometimes (in extreme cases) contradict known findings in cognitive psychology.[10]

He was also an early leader in research on intelligent tutoring systems, such as cognitive tutors, and many of Anderson's former students, such as Kenneth Koedinger and Neil Heffernan, have become leaders in that area.

Intelligent tutoring systems edit

Anderson's research has used fMRI brain imaging to study how students learn with intelligent tutoring systems.[11] Most of his studies have looked at neural processes of students while they are solving algebraic equations or proofs.

Anderson and colleagues generated a cognitive model that predicted that while students were learning an algebra proof, neuroimages showed decreased activation in a lateral inferior prefrontal region and a predefined fusiform region. This decrease in activity showed an increased fluency in retrieving declarative information, as students required less activity in these regions to solve the problems.[11]

Cognitive stages when solving mathematical problems edit

In a 2012 study, Anderson and Jon Fincham, a colleague at Carnegie Mellon, examined the cognitive stages participants engaged in when solving mathematical problems. These stages included encoding, planning, solving, and response. The study determined how much time participants spent in each problem solving stage when presented with a mathematical problem. Multi-voxel pattern recognition techniques and Hidden Markov models were used to determine participants' problem solving stages.

The results of the study showed that the time spent in the planning stage was dependent on the novelty of the problem. The time spent in the solving stage was dependent on the amount of computation required for the particular problem. Lastly, the time spent in the response stage was dependent on the complexity of the response required by the problem.[12]

Decomposition Hypothesis edit

In another study, Anderson and colleagues used a video game task to test the Decomposition Hypothesis, or the idea that a complex cognitive task can be broken down into a set of information processing components. The combination of these components remains the same across different tasks. The study used a cognitive model that predicted behavioral and activation patterns for specific regions in the brain.

The predictions involved both tonic activation, which remained stable across brain regions during game play, and phasic activation, which was present only when there was resource competition. The study's results supported the Decomposition Hypothesis. Individual differences were also found in participants' learning gains, which indicated that the rate of learning for a complex skill is dependent on cognitive capacity limits.[13]

Publications edit

  • 1976. Language, memory, and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • 1980. Cognitive psychology and its implications. San Francisco: Freeman. Eighth edition, Worth Publishers, 2014. ISBN 978-1464148910
  • 1983. The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • 1990. The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • 2000. Learning and Memory: An Integrated Approach. Wiley. ISBN 978-0471249252
  • 2007. How can the human mind occur in the physical universe? New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0195398953

References edit

  1. ^ National Academy of Sciences: Anderson, John R.
  2. ^ "Book of Members, 1780–2010: Chapter A" (PDF). American Academy of Arts and Sciences. Retrieved April 18, 2011.
  3. ^ See the list of Rumelhart prize winners on the Cognitive Science Society website Archived December 18, 2007, at the Wayback Machine.
  4. ^ See the list of prize winners at the Heineken Prize page website.
  5. ^ "Benjamin Franklin Medal in Computer and Cognitive Science". Franklin Institute. 2011. Archived from the original on July 31, 2012. Retrieved December 23, 2011.
  6. ^ Anderson, J. R. & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  7. ^ Anderson, J. R. (2007). How can the human mind occur in the physical universe? New York: Oxford University Press
  8. ^ Anderson, J. R. (1991). Is human cognition adaptive? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14, 471–517.
  9. ^ Anderson, J. R. (1990). "The adaptive character of thought". Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  10. ^ Anderson, John R.; Reder, Lynne M.; Simon, Herbert A.; K. Anders Ericsson; Robert Glaser (1998). Diane Ravitch (ed.). "Radical constructivism and cognitive psychology". Brookings Papers on Education Policy. 6 (1): 227–278..
  11. ^ a b "ACT-R » Publications » Can Neural Imaging Investigate Learning in an Educational Task?".
  12. ^ "Discovering the Sequential Structure of Thought"
  13. ^ "ACT-R » Publications » Brain Regions Engaged by Part- and Whole-task Performance in a Video Game: A Model-based Test of the Decomposition Hypothesis".

External links edit

  • Personal page at CMU
  • Biography