Odor

Summary

An odor (American English) or odour (Commonwealth English; see spelling differences) is caused by one or more volatilized chemical compounds that are generally found in low concentrations that humans and many animals can perceive via their sense of smell. An odor is also called a "smell" or a "scent", which can refer to either an unpleasant or a pleasant odor.

"Smell", from Allegory of the Senses by Jan Brueghel the Elder, Museo del Prado

While "odor" and "smell" can refer to pleasant and unpleasant odors, the terms "scent", "aroma", and "fragrance" are usually reserved for pleasant-smelling odors and are frequently used in the food and cosmetic industry to describe floral scents or to refer to perfumes.

Physiology of smell edit

 
Odor control covers at a sewage treatment plant: Under these covers, grit and gravel are settled out of the wastewater.

Sense of smell edit

The perception of odors, or sense of smell, is mediated by the olfactory nerve. The olfactory receptor (OR) cells are neurons present in the olfactory epithelium, which is a small patch of tissue at the back of the nasal cavity. There are millions of olfactory receptor neurons that act as sensory signaling cells. Each neuron has cilia in direct contact with the air. Odorous molecules bind to receptor proteins extending from cilia and act as a chemical stimulus, initiating electric signals that travel along the olfactory nerve's axons to the brain.[1]

When an electrical signal reaches a threshold, the neuron fires, which sends a signal traveling along the axon to the olfactory bulb, a part of the limbic system of the brain. Interpretation of the smell begins there, relating the smell to past experiences and in relation to the substance(s) inhaled. The olfactory bulb acts as a relay station connecting the nose to the olfactory cortex in the brain. Olfactory information is further processed and forwarded to the central nervous system (CNS), which controls emotions and behavior as well as basic thought processes.

Odor sensation usually depends on the concentration (number of molecules) available to the olfactory receptors. A single odorant is usually recognized by many receptors. Different odorants are recognized by combinations of receptors. The patterns of neuron signals help to identify the smell. The olfactory system does not interpret a single compound, but instead the whole odorous mix. This does not correspond to the concentration or intensity of any single constituent.[2][3]

Most odors consist of organic compounds, although some simple compounds not containing carbon, such as hydrogen sulfide and ammonia, are also odorants. The perception of an odor effect is a two-step process. First, there is the physiological part. This is the detection of stimuli by receptors in the nose. The stimuli are recognized by the region of the human brain which handles olfaction. Because of this, an objective and analytical measure of odor is impossible. While odor feelings are personal perceptions, individual reactions are usually related. They relate to things such as gender, age, state of health, and personal history.

Smell acuity by age and sex edit

The ability to identify odor varies among people and decreases with age. Studies claim that there are sex differences in odor discrimination, and that women usually outperform men.[4] Conversely, there are some studies claiming a male advantage.[5][6][7] A 2019 meta-analysis claimed that the differences in olfaction are extremely small, but confirmed a small advantage for women.[8]

Pregnant women have increased smell sensitivity, sometimes resulting in abnormal taste and smell perceptions, leading to food cravings or aversions.[9] The ability to taste also decreases with age as the sense of smell tends to dominate the sense of taste. Chronic smell problems are reported in small numbers for those in their mid-twenties, with numbers increasing steadily, with overall sensitivity beginning to decline in the second decade of life, and then deteriorating appreciably as age increases, especially once over 70 years of age.[10]

Smell acuity compared to other animals edit

For most untrained individuals, the act of smelling acquires little information concerning the specific ingredients of an odor. Their smell perception primarily offers information that elicits an emotional response.[citation needed] Experienced individuals, however, such as flavorists and perfumers, can identify discrete chemicals in complex mixtures using only their sense of smell.

Odor perception is a primary evolutionary sense. The sense of smell can induce pleasure or subconsciously warn of danger, which may, for example, help to locate mates, find food, or detect predators. Humans have an unusually good sense of smell considering they have only 350 functional olfactory receptor genes compared to the 1,300 found in mice, for example. This is despite an apparent evolutionary decline in the sense of smell.[11][12] The human sense of smell is comparable with many animals, able to distinguish between a diverse range of odors. Studies have reported that humans can distinguish in the region of one trillion unique aromas.[13][14]

Habituation or adaptation edit

Odors that a person is used to, such as their own body odor, are less noticeable than uncommon odors. This is due to "habituation." After continuous odor exposure, the sense of smell is fatigued, but recovers if the stimulus is removed for a time.[15] Odors can change due to environmental conditions: for example, odors tend to be more distinguishable in cool dry air.[16]

Habituation affects the ability to distinguish odors after continuous exposure. The sensitivity and ability to discriminate odors diminishes with exposure, and the brain tends to ignore continuous stimulus and focus on differences and changes in a particular sensation. When odorants are mixed, a habitual odorant is blocked. This depends on the strength of the odorants in the mixture, which can change the perception and processing of an odor. This process helps classify similar odors as well as adjust sensitivity to differences in complex stimuli.[17]

Genetic component edit

The primary gene sequences for thousands of olfactory receptors are known for the genomes of more than a dozen organisms. They are seven-helix-turn transmembrane proteins. But there are no known structures for any olfactory receptor. There is a conserved sequence in roughly three quarters of all ORs. This is a tripodal metal-ion binding site,[18] and Suslick has proposed that the ORs are in fact metalloproteins (most likely with zinc, copper, and manganese ions) that serve as a Lewis Acid site for the binding of many odorant molecules. In 1978, Crabtree suggested that Cu(I) is "the most likely candidate for a metallo-receptor site in olfaction" of strong-smelling volatiles. These are also good metal-coordinating ligands, such as thiols.[19] In 2012, Zhuang, Matsunami, and Block confirmed the Crabtree/Suslick proposal for the specific case of a mouse OR, MOR244-3, showing that copper is essential for detection of certain thiols and other sulfur-containing compounds. Thus, by using a chemical that binds to copper in the mouse nose, so that copper wasn't available to the receptors, the authors showed that the mice couldn't detect the thiols without the copper. However, these authors also found that MOR244-3 lacks the specific metal ion binding site suggested by Suslick, instead showing a different motif in the EC2 domain.[20]

Evolutionary impact edit

Gordon Shepherd proposed that the retro-nasal route of olfaction (odorants introduced to the olfactory mucosa through the oral cavity often as food) was partially responsible for the development of human olfactory acuity. He suggested the evolutionary pressure of diversification of food sources and increased complexity of food preparation presented humans with a broader range of odorants, ultimately leading to a "richer repertoire of smells". Animals such as dogs show a greater sensitivity to odors than humans, especially in studies using short-chain compounds. Higher cognitive brain mechanisms and more olfactory brain regions enable humans to discriminate odors better than other mammals despite fewer olfactory receptor genes.[21]

Measuring techniques edit

Concentration edit

Odor concentration refers to an odor's pervasiveness. To measure odor sensation, an odor is diluted to a detection or recognition threshold. The detection threshold is the concentration of an odor in air when 50% of a population can distinguish between the odorous sample and an odor-free reference sample. The recognition odor threshold is usually a factor of two to five higher than the detection threshold.[22]

The measurement of odor concentration is the most widespread method to quantify odors. It is standardized in CEN EN 13725:2003.[23] The method is based on dilution of an odor sample to the odor threshold. The numerical value of the odor concentration is equal to the dilution factor that is necessary to reach the odor threshold. Its unit is the "European Odour Unit", OUE. Therefore, the odor concentration at the odor threshold is 1 OUE by definition.

Olfactometer edit

To establish odor concentration, an olfactometer is used which employs a group of human panelists. A diluted odorous mixture and an odor-free gas—n-Butanol—as a reference are presented from sniffing ports to a group of panelists who are sensitive in odor perception. To collect an odor sample, the samples are collected using specialized sample bags, which are made from an odor free material, e.g., Teflon. The most accepted technique for collecting odor samples is the lung technique, where the sample bag is placed in a sealed drum, where a vacuum is created outside the bag, which fills under expansion, and draws into itself the sample from the source. Critically, all components which touch the odor sample, must be odor free, which includes lines and fittings.

In comparing the odor emitted from each port, the panelists are asked to report if they can detect a difference between the ports. The gas-diluting ratio is then decreased by a factor of 1.4 or two (i.e., the concentration is increased accordingly). The panelists are asked to repeat the test. This continues until the panelists respond with certainty and correctly twice in a row. These responses are used to calculate the concentration of the odor in terms of European odor units (OUE/m3, where 1 OUE/m3≡40 ppb/v n-butanol).[24]

Humans can discriminate between two odorants that differ in concentration by as little as 7%.[25] A human's odor detection threshold is variable. Repeated exposure to an odorant leads to enhanced olfactory sensitivity and decreased detection thresholds for a number of different odorants.[26] It was found in a study that humans who were unable to detect the odor of androstenone developed the ability to detect it after repeated exposure.[27] People who cannot smell are said to be anosmic.

There are a number of issues which have to be overcome with sampling, these include:

  1. If the source is under vacuum
  2. if the source is at a high temperature
  3. If the source has high humidity

Issues such as temperature and humidity are best overcome using either pre-dilution or dynamic dilution techniques.

Other analytical methods edit

Other analytic methods can be subdivided into the physical, the gas chromatographical, and the chemosensory method.

When measuring odor, there is a difference between emission and immission measurements. Emission measurement can be taken by olfactometry using an olfactometer to dilute the odor sample. Olfactometry is rarely used for immission measurement because of low odor concentrations involved. The same measuring principles are used, but the judgment of the air-assay happens without diluting the samples.

Odor measurement is essential for odor regulation and control.[28] An odor emission often consists of a complex mixture of many odorous compounds. Analytical monitoring of individual chemical compounds present in such an odor is usually not practical. As a result, odor sensory methods, instead of instrumental methods, are normally used to measure such odor. Odor sensory methods are available to monitor odor both from source emissions and in the ambient air. These two contexts require different approaches for measuring odor. The collection of odor samples is more easily accomplished for a source emission than for odor in the ambient air.[29]

Field measurement with portable field olfactometers can seem more effective, but olfactometer use is not regulated in Europe, while it is popular in the U.S. and Canada, where several states set limits at the receptor sites or along the perimeter of odor-emitting plants, expressed in units of dilution-to-threshold (D/T).[30]

Intensity edit

Odor intensity is the perceived strength of odor sensation. This intensity property is used to locate the source of odors and perhaps most directly related to odor nuisance.[3]

The perceived strength of the odor sensation is measured in conjunction with odor concentration. This can be modeled by the Weber-Fechner law: I = a × log(c) + b,[31] where I is the perceived psychological intensity at the dilution step on the butanol scale, a is the Weber-Fechner coefficient, C is the chemical concentrations, and b is the intercept constant (0.5 by definition).[31]

Odor intensity can be expressed using an odor intensity scale, which is a verbal description of an odor sensation to which a numerical value is assigned.[31]

Odor intensity can be divided into the following categories according to intensity:

0 – no odor
1 – very weak (odor threshold)
2 – weak
3 – distinct
4 – strong
5 – very strong
6 – intolerable

Odor intensity is determined in a laboratory by specialists who have been trained to accurately define intensity.

Hedonic tone assessment edit

Hedonic assessment is the process of rating odors according to a scale ranging from extremely unpleasant to extremely pleasant. Intensity and hedonic tone, whilst similar, refer to different things: that is, the strength of the odor (intensity) and the pleasantness of an odor (hedonic tone). The perception of an odor may change from pleasant to unpleasant with increasing concentration, intensity, time, frequency, or previous experience with a specific odor—all factors in determining a response.[32]

FIDOL factors edit

The overall set of qualities are sometimes identified as the "FIDOL (Frequency, Intensity, Duration, Offensiveness, Location) factors".[33]

The character of an odor is a critical element in assessing an odor. This property is the ability to distinguish different odors and is only descriptive. First, a basic description is used—such as sweet, pungent, acrid, fragrant, warm, dry, or sour. The odor is then referenced to a source such as sewage or apple which can then be followed by a reference to a specific chemical such as acids or gasoline.[3]

Most commonly, a set of standard descriptors is used, which may range from "fragrant" to "sewer odor".[34] Although the method is fairly simplistic, it is important for the FIDOL factors to be understood by the person rating the smell. This method is most commonly used to define the character of an odor which can then be compared to other odors. It is common for olfactometry laboratories to report character as an additional factor post sample-analysis.

Categorization edit

Different categorizations of primary odors have been proposed, including the following, which identifies 7 primary odors:[22][35][36]

  1. Musky – perfumes
  2. Putrid – rotten eggs
  3. Pungent – vinegar
  4. Camphoraceousmothballs
  5. Etherealdry cleaning fluid
  6. Floral – roses (see also floral scent)
  7. Pepperminty – mint gum

Though the concept of primary odors is not universally accepted.[36]

Interpretive dispersion modeling edit

In many countries odor modeling is used to determine the extent of an impact from an odor source. These are a function of modeled concentration, averaging time (over what time period the model steps are run over, typically hourly), and a percentile. Percentiles refer to a statistical representation of how many hours per year the concentration C may be exceeded based on the averaging period.

Sampling from area sources edit

There are two main odor sampling techniques: direct and indirect odor sampling techniques.

Direct sampling edit

Direct refers to the placement of an enclosure on or over an emitting surface from which samples are collected, and an odor emission rate is determined.

The most commonly used direct methods include the flux chamber[37] and wind tunnels such as the one at the University of New South Wales (UNSW).[38] There are many other available techniques, and consideration should be given to a number of factors before selecting a suitable method.

A source which has implications for this method are sources, such as bark bed biofilters, that have a vertical velocity component. For such sources, consideration must be given as to the most appropriate method. A commonly used technique is to measure the odor concentration at the emitting surface, and combine this with the volumetric flow rate of air entering the biofilter to produce an emission rate.

Indirect sampling edit

Indirect sampling is often referred to as back calculation. It involves the use of a mathematical formula to predict an emission rate.

Many methods are used, but all make use of the same inputs which include surface roughness, upwind and downwind concentrations, stability class (or other similar factor), wind speed, and wind direction.

Health risks edit

The human sense of smell is a primary factor in the sensation of comfort. Olfaction as a sensory system brings awareness of the presence of airborne chemicals. Some inhaled chemicals are volatile compounds that act as a stimulus, triggering unwanted reactions such as nose, eye, and throat irritation. Perception of odor and of irritation is unique to each person, and varies because of physical conditions or memory of past exposures to similar chemicals. A person's specific threshold, before an odor becomes a nuisance, depends also on the frequency, concentration, and duration of an odor.

The perception of irritation from odor sensation is hard to investigate because exposure to a volatile chemical elicits a different response based on sensory and physiological signals, and interpretation of these signals is influenced by experience, expectations, personality, or situational factors. Volatile organic compounds (VOCs) may have higher concentrations in confined indoor environments, due to restricted infiltration of fresh air, as compared to the outdoor environment, leading to greater potential for toxic health exposures from a variety of chemical compounds. Health effects of odor are traced to the sensation of an odor or the odorant itself. Health effects and symptoms vary—including eye, nose, or throat irritation, cough, chest tightness, drowsiness, and mood change—all of which decrease as an odor ceases. Odors may also trigger illnesses such as asthma, depression, stress-induced illness, or hypersensitivity. The ability to perform tasks may decrease, and other social/behavioral changes may occur.

Occupants should expect remediation from disturbing and unexpected odors that disturb concentration, diminish productivity, evoke symptoms, and generally increase the dislike for a particular environment. It is important to set occupational exposure limits (OELs) to ensure the health and safety of workers, as well as comfort, because exposure to chemicals can elicit physiological and biochemical changes in the upper respiratory system. Standards are hard to set when exposures are not reported and can also be hard to measure. Workforce populations vary in terms of discomfort from odors because of exposure history or habituation, and they may not realize possible risks of exposure to chemicals that produce specific odors.[39][40]

Types edit

Some odors are sought after, such as from perfumes and flowers, some of which command high prices. Whole industries have developed around products that remove or mask unpleasant odors, such as deodorant.

Odor molecules transmit messages to the limbic system, the area of the brain that governs emotional responses. Some believe that these messages have the power to alter moods, evoke distant memories, raise spirits, and boost self-confidence. This belief has led to "aromatherapy", wherein fragrances are claimed to cure a wide range of psychological and physical problems. Aromatherapy claims that fragrances can positively affect sleep, stress, alertness, social interaction, and general feelings of well-being. Evidence for the effectiveness of aromatherapy is mostly anecdotal and controlled scientific studies to substantiate its claims are lacking.

Some people are allergic to the fragrances found in perfume, scented shampoo, scented deodorant, or similar products. Reactions, as with other chemical allergies, can range from slight headaches to anaphylactic shock, which can result in death.[citation needed]

Unpleasant odors play various roles in nature, often to warn of danger, though this may not be known to the subject who smells it.[41] The natural gas industry uses odor to enable consumers to identify leaks. Natural gas in its native state is colorless and almost odorless. To help users detect leaks, an odorizer with the scent of rotten eggs, tert-Butylthiol (t-butyl mercaptan), is added. Sometimes a related compound, thiophane, may be used in the mixture.

An odor that is viewed as unpleasant by some people or cultures can be viewed as attractive by others where it is more familiar or has a better reputation.[41] It is commonly thought that those exuding an unpleasant body odor are unattractive to others. But studies have shown that a person who is exposed to a particular unpleasant odor can be attracted to others who have been exposed to the same unpleasant odor.[41] This includes odors associated with pollution.[41]

What causes a substance to smell unpleasant may be different from what one perceives. For example, perspiration is often viewed as having an unpleasant odor, but it is odorless. It is bacteria in the perspiration that causes the odor.[42]

Unpleasant odors can arise from specific industrial processes, adversely affecting workers and even residents downwind of the source. The most common sources of industrial odor arise from sewage treatment plants, refineries, animal rendering factories, and industries processing chemicals (such as sulfur) which have odorous characteristics. Sometimes industrial odor sources are the subject of community controversy and scientific analysis.

Body odor is present both in animals and humans and its intensity can be influenced by many factors (behavioral patterns, survival strategies). Body odor has a strong genetic basis both in animals and humans, but it can be also strongly influenced by various diseases and psychological conditions.

Study edit

The study of odors is a growing field but is a complex and difficult one. The human olfactory system can detect many thousands of scents based on only minute airborne concentrations of a chemical. The sense of smell of many animals is even better. Some fragrant flowers give off odor plumes that move downwind and are detectable by bees more than a kilometer away.

The study of odors is complicated by the complex chemistry taking place at the moment of a smell sensation. For example, iron-containing metallic objects are perceived to have a distinctive odor when touched, although iron's vapor pressure is negligible. According to a 2006 study, this smell is the result of aldehydes (for example, nonanal) and ketones: 1-octen-3-one) released from the human skin on contact with ferrous ions that are formed in the sweat-mediated corrosion of iron. The same chemicals are also associated with the smell of blood, as ferrous iron in blood on skin produces the same reaction.[43]

Pheromones edit

Pheromones are odors that are used for communication, and are sometimes called "airborne hormones". A female moth may release a pheromone that can entice a male moth that is several kilometers downwind. Honeybee queens constantly release pheromones that regulate the activity of the hive. Worker bees can release such smells to call other bees into an appropriate cavity when a swarm moves into new quarters, or to "sound" an alarm when the hive is threatened.

Advanced technology edit

Most artificial or electronic nose instruments work by combining output from an array of non-specific chemical sensors to produce a fingerprint of whatever volatile chemicals in the local environment.[44] Most electronic noses need to be "trained" to recognize chemicals of interest before it can be used.[45][46] Many current electronic-nose instruments suffer from problems with reproducibility subject to varying ambient temperature and humidity. An example of this type of technology is the colorimetric sensor array, which visualizes odor through color change and creates a "picture" of it.[47][48]

Behavioral cues edit

Odor perception is a complex process involving the central nervous system and can evoke psychological and physiological responses. Because the olfactory signal terminates in or near the amygdala, odors are strongly linked to memories and can evoke emotions. The amygdala participates in the hedonic or emotional processing of olfactory stimuli.[49] Odors can disturb our concentration, diminish productivity, evoke symptoms, and in general increase a dislike for an environment. Odors can impact the liking for a person, place, food, or product as a form of conditioning.[50] Memories recalled by odors are significantly more emotional and evocative than those recalled by the same cue presented visually or auditorily.[51] Odors can become conditioned to experiential states and when later encountered have directional influences on behavior. Doing a frustrating task in a scented room decreases performance of other cognitive tasks in the presence of the same odor.[52] Nonhuman animals communicate their emotional states through changes in body odor, and human body odors are indicative of emotional state.[53]

Human body odors influence interpersonal relationships and are involved in adaptive behaviors, such as parental attachment in infants or partner choice in adults. "Mothers can discriminate the odor of their own child, and infants recognize and prefer the body odor of their mother over that of another woman. This maternal odor appears to guide infants toward the breast and to have a calming effect."[citation needed] Body odor is involved in the development of infant–mother attachment and is essential to a child's social and emotional development and evokes feelings of security. Reassurance created by familiar parental body odors may contribute significantly to the attachment process.[54] Human body odors can also affect mate choice. Fragrances are commonly used to raise sexual attractiveness and induce sexual arousal. Researchers found that people choose perfume that interacts well with their body odor.[55]

Body odor is a sensory cue critical for mate selection in humans because it is a signal of immunological health. Women prefer men with major histocompatibility complex (MHC) genotypes and odor different from themselves especially during ovulation. Different MHC alleles are favorable because different allele combinations would maximize disease protection and minimize recessive mutations in offspring. Biologically females tend to select mates "who are most likely to secure offspring survival and thus increase the likelihood that her genetic contribution will be reproductively viable."[56]

Studies have suggested that people might be using odor cues associated with the immune system to select mates. Using a brain-imaging technique, Swedish researchers have shown that gay and straight males' brains respond in different ways to two odors that may be involved in sexual arousal, and that the gay men respond in the same way as straight women, though it could not be determined whether this was cause or effect. The study was expanded to include lesbian women; the results were consistent with previous findings that lesbian women were not as responsive to male-identified odors, while their response to female cues was similar to that of straight males.[57] According to the researchers, this research suggests a possible role for human pheromones in the biological basis of sexual orientation.[58]

An odor can cue recall of a distant memory. Most memories that pertain to odor come from the first decade of life, compared to verbal and visual memories which usually come from the 10th to 30th years of life.[59] Odor-evoked memories are more emotional, associated with stronger feelings of being brought back in time, and have been thought of less often as compared to memories evoked by other cues.[59]

Use in design edit

The sense of smell is not overlooked as a way of marketing products. The deliberate and controlled application of scent is used by designers, scientists, artists, perfumers, architects, and chefs. Some applications of scents in environments are in casinos, hotels, private clubs, and new automobiles. For example, "technicians at New York City’s Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center disperse vanilla-scented oil into the air to help patients cope with the claustrophobic effects of MRI testing. Scents are used at the Chicago Board of Trade to lower the decibel level on the trading floor."[60]

If ingredients are listed on a product, the term "fragrance" can be used in a general sense.

Scent preferences edit

Effect of perfume on sexual attractiveness edit

Both men and women use perfume to boost their sexual attractiveness to members of the opposite, or same, sex. When people find that a particular perfume or aftershave is perceived positively, they may be hard-pressed to change it. Olfactory communication is natural in humans. Without perfume or aftershave, humans unconsciously detect people's natural scents: in the form of pheromones. Pheromones are usually detected unconsciously, and it is believed that they have an important influence on human's social and sexual behaviour [61] There are a number of hypotheses concerning why humans wear perfume or aftershave, and whether it amplifies or reduces their natural scents.

In 2001, a study found that the major histocompatibility complex (MHC) (a polymorphic set of genes which is important for immune-function in humans) is correlated with the ingredients found in perfume. This suggests that humans do, in fact, choose perfumes that complement or enhance their natural scents (their pheromones). This evidence offers support for the hypothesis that perfume is chosen by individuals to advertise their physical health. Research suggests that this advertisement of good health will, in fact, enhance females’ attractiveness to the opposite sex as health markers have been shown to do.[62] While strong evidence has been found to support the hypothesis that wearing perfume enhances females’ attractiveness to males, little research has been done into the effect of fragrance on males’ attractiveness to females. Considerably more research has covered the effect of males’ natural odor and females’ ratings of attractiveness. Many studies[63] found that odor predicted attractiveness when female raters were not on any form of contraceptive pill. For those who were, there was no relation between attractiveness and body odor.

A person's odor can increase or decrease ratings of attractiveness because the olfactory receptors in the brain are directly linked with the limbic system, the part of the brain that is thought to be most involved with emotion. This link is important, because if an individual associates positive affect (elicited by pheromones[64]), with a potential mate, their liking for, and attraction to, that potential mate will be increased.[65] Although not a typically evolutionary hypothesis, this hypothesis is one that acknowledges how humans have adapted their mating strategies to modern-day societal norms.

Major histocompatibility complex (MHC) and body odor preferences edit

Major histocompatibility complex (MHC) is a genotype found in vertebrates, including humans. MHC is thought to contribute to mate choice in animals and humans. In sexual selection, females opt for mates with an MHC which differs from their own, optimising genes for their offspring.[66] The "heterozygote advantage" and "Red Queen" explanations for these findings fall under the "pathogen hypothesis". Due to differences in MHC alleles' resistance to pathogens, a preference for mates with a dissimilar MHC composition has been argued to act as a mechanism to avoid infectious diseases. According to the heterozygotes-advantage hypothesis, diversity within the MHC genotype is beneficial for the immune system due to a greater range of antigens available to the host. Therefore, the hypothesis proposes that MHC heterozygotes will be superior to MHC homozygotes in fighting off pathogens. Experimental research has shown mixed findings for this idea.[67] The "Red Queen" or "rare-allele" hypothesis suggests that diversity in the MHC gene provides a moving target for pathogens, making it more difficult for them to adapt to MHC genotypes in the host.[68] Another hypothesis suggests that preferences for MHC-dissimilar mates could serve to avoid inbreeding.[69]

Body odor can provide MHC information. Although less is known about how odor is influenced by MHC genes, possible explanations have been that microbial flora[70] or volatile acids[71] are affected by the gene, which can be detected in body odor. Female mice and humans have both shown odor preferences for males with MHC-dissimilarity.[72] Research has shown that women prefer the scent of men with dissimilar MHC genes. In a study, women rated the scent of T-shirts, worn over two nights by men, as more pleasant when smelling those of MHC-dissimilar men.[73] It has also been found that women were reminded more of current or prior partners when smelling odors from men whose MHC was dissimilar to their own. A study of married couples found that MHC haplotypes differed between spouses more than chance would dictate.[74] Taking oral contraceptives has been found to reverse the MHC-dissimilarity odor preference.[75]

Women's scent preferences and the menstrual cycle edit

Women's preferences for body odor change with their menstrual cycles.[76] The ovulatory-shift hypothesis argues that women experience elevated immediate sexual attraction, relative to low-fertile days of the cycle, to men with characteristics that reflect good genetic quality.[77] Body odor may provide significant cues about a potential sexual partner's genetic quality, reproductive status, and health, with a woman's preferences for particular body odors becoming heightened during her most fertile days.[78] As certain body odors can reflect good genetic quality, woman are more likely to prefer these scents when they are fertile, as this is when they are most likely to produce offspring with any potential mates, with conception-risk being related to a preference for the scent of male symmetry.[76] Men also prefer the scent of women at their fertile cycle points.[79]

There are several scents that reflect good genetic quality that females prefer during the most fertile phase of their cycles. Women prefer the scent of symmetrical men more during the fertile phases of their menstrual cycle than during their infertile phases,[80] with estrogen positively predicting women's preferences for the scent of symmetry.[81] Women's preferences for masculine faces is greatest when their fertility is at its highest,[80] and so is the preference for attractive faces.[82] Other scents found to be preferred by women in the most fertile phase of their cycle are the scent for developmental stability,[83] and the scent for dominance.[84]

If women are taking a contraceptive pill the changes in mate scent preferences over the menstrual cycle are not expressed.[85] If odor plays a role in human mate choice then the contraceptive pill could disrupt disassortative mate preferences.[86] Those taking a contraceptive pill show no significant preference for the scent of either symmetrical or asymmetrical men, whereas normally cycling women prefer the scent of shirts worn by symmetrical men.[87] Males' preferences for women's scent may also change if the woman is taking oral contraceptives. When women take a contraceptive pill, this has been found to demolish the cycle attractiveness of odors that men find attractive in normally ovulating women.[88] Therefore, a contraceptive pill affects both women's preferences for scent and also affects their own scents, making their scent less attractive to males than the scent of normally cycling women.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ de march, Claire A.; Ryu, sangEun; Sicard, Gilles; Moon, Cheil; Golebiowski, Jérôme (September 2015). "Structure–odour relationships reviewed in the postgenomic era". Flavour and Fragrance Journal. 30 (5): 342–61. doi:10.1002/ffj.3249.
  2. ^ Axel, Richard (1995). "The molecular logic of smell". Scientific American. 273 (4): 154–59. Bibcode:1995SciAm.273d.154A. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican1095-154. PMID 7481719.
  3. ^ a b c Spengler, p. 492
  4. ^ Doty, Richard L.; Applebaum, Steven; Zusho, Hiroyuki; Settle, R.Gregg (1985). "Sex differences in odor identification ability: A cross-cultural analysis". Neuropsychologia. 23 (5): 667–72. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(85)90067-3. PMID 4058710. S2CID 43729693.
  5. ^ Bailey, E. H. S.; Powell, L. M. (1883). "Some Special Tests in Regard to the Delicacy of the Sense of Smell". Transactions of the Annual Meetings of the Kansas Academy of Science. 9: 100–101. doi:10.2307/3623630. ISSN 1933-0545. JSTOR 3623630.
  6. ^ AMOORE, JOHN E.; VENSTROM, DELPHA (January 1966). "Sensory Analysis of Odor Qualities in Terms of the Stereochemical Theory". Journal of Food Science. 31 (1): 118–128. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2621.1966.tb15424.x. ISSN 0022-1147.
  7. ^ VENSTROM, DELPHA; AMOORE, JOHN E. (May 1968). "Olfactory Threshold, in Relation to Age, Sex or Smoking". Journal of Food Science. 33 (3): 264–265. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2621.1968.tb01364.x. ISSN 0022-1147.
  8. ^ Sorokowski, Piotr; Karwowski, Maciej; Misiak, Michał; Marczak, Michalina Konstancja; Dziekan, Martyna; Hummel, Thomas; Sorokowska, Agnieszka (2019). "Sex Differences in Human Olfaction: A Meta-Analysis". Frontiers in Psychology. 10: 242. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00242. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 6381007. PMID 30814965.
  9. ^ Nordin, Steven; Broman, Daniel A.; Olofsson, Jonas K.; Wulff, Marianne (2004). "A Longitudinal Descriptive Study of Self-reported Abnormal Smell and Taste Perception". Pregnant Women Chem. Senses. 29 (5): 391–402. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh040. PMID 15201206.
  10. ^ Hoffman, H. J.; Cruickshanks, K. J.; Davis, B (2009). "Perspectives on population-based epidemiological studies of olfactory and taste impairment". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1170 (1): 514–30. Bibcode:2009NYASA1170..514H. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04597.x. PMC 2760342. PMID 19686188.
  11. ^ Rouquier, S; Blancher, A; Giorgi, D (2000). "The olfactory receptor gene repertoire in primates and mouse: Evidence for reduction of the functional fraction in primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. PNAS. 97 (6): 2870–2874. Bibcode:2000PNAS...97.2870R. doi:10.1073/pnas.040580197. PMC 16022. PMID 10706615.
  12. ^ Gilad, Y; Przeworski, M; Lancet, D (2004). "Loss of olfactory receptor genes coincides with the acquisition of full trichromatic vision in primates". PLOS Biology. Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany. gilad@eva.mpg.de. 2 (1): e5. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020005. PMC 314465. PMID 14737185.
  13. ^ Bushdid, C.; Magnasco, M. O.; Vosshall, L. B.; Keller, A. (March 21, 2014). "Humans Can Discriminate More than 1 Trillion Olfactory Stimuli". Science. 343 (6177): 1370–72. Bibcode:2014Sci...343.1370B. doi:10.1126/science.1249168. PMC 4483192. PMID 24653035.
  14. ^ Kean, Sam (2017). "The Scent of a Molecule". Distillations. 3 (3): 5. Retrieved May 16, 2018.
  15. ^ Chaudhury, D; Manella, L; Arellanos, A; Escanilla, O; Cleland, T. A.; Linster, C (2010). "Olfactory bulb habituation to odor stimuli". Behavioral Neuroscience. 124 (4): 490–99. doi:10.1037/a0020293. PMC 2919830. PMID 20695648.
  16. ^ Salthammer, Tunga; Bahadir, Müfit (2009). "Occurrence, Dynamics and Reactions of Organic Pollutants in the Indoor Environment". CLEAN – Soil, Air, Water. 37 (6): 417–35. doi:10.1002/clen.200900015.
  17. ^ Devriese, S; Winters, W; Stegen, K; Diest, I Van; Veulemans, H; Nemery, B; Eelen, P (2000). "Generalization of acquired somatic symptoms in response to odors: a pavlovian perspective on multiple chemical sensitivity". Psychosom. Med. 62 (6): 751–59. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.169. doi:10.1097/00006842-200011000-00003. PMID 11138993. S2CID 12093019.
  18. ^ Wang, J.; Luthey-Schulten, Z.; Suslick, K. S. (2003). "Is the Olfactory Receptor A Metalloprotein?". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100 (6): 3035–39. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.3035W. doi:10.1073/pnas.262792899. PMC 152240. PMID 12610211.
  19. ^ Crabtree, R.H. (1978). "Copper(I) – Possible Olfactory Binding-Site". J. Inorg. Nucl. Chem. 1978 (40): 1453. doi:10.1016/0022-1902(78)80071-2.
  20. ^ Duan, Xufang; Block, Eric; Li, Zhen; Connelly, Timothy; Zhang, Jian; Huang, Zhimin; Su, Xubo; Pan, Yi; Wu, Lifang; Chi, Qiuyi; Thomas, Siji; Zhang, Shaozhong; Ma, Minghong; Matsunami, Hiroaki; Chen, Guo-Qiang; Zhuang, Hanyi (2012). "Crucial role of copper in detection of metal-coordinating odorants". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 (9): 3492–97. Bibcode:2012PNAS..109.3492D. doi:10.1073/pnas.1111297109. PMC 3295281. PMID 22328155.
  21. ^ Shepherd, Gordon M. (2004). "The Human Sense of Smell: Are We Better Than We Think?". PLOS Biology. 2 (5): e146. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020146. PMC 406401. PMID 15138509.
  22. ^ a b Spengler, p. 483
  23. ^ CEN EN 13725:2003, Air quality – Determination of odour concentration by dynamic olfactometry Archived 2015-05-05 at the Wayback Machine. sipe-rtd.info
  24. ^ Van Harreveld, A. P.; Heeres, P.; Harssema, H. (1999). "A review of 20 years of standardization of odor concentration measurement by dynamic olfactometry in Europe". Journal of the Air & Waste Management Association. 49 (6): 705–15. doi:10.1080/10473289.1999.11499900. PMID 29073832.
  25. ^ Cain, WS. (1977). "Differential sensitivity for smell: "noise" at the nose". Science. 195 (4280): 796–98. Bibcode:1977Sci...195..796C. doi:10.1126/science.836592. PMID 836592.
  26. ^ Cain, W. S.; Gent, J. F. (1991). "Olfactory sensitivity: Reliability, generality, and association with aging". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 17 (2): 382–91. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.17.2.382. PMID 1830082.
  27. ^ Wysocki, C.J.; Dorries, K.M.; Beauchamp, G.K. (1989). "Ability to perceive androstenone can be acquired by ostensibly anosmic people". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 86 (20): 7976–78. Bibcode:1989PNAS...86.7976W. doi:10.1073/pnas.86.20.7976. PMC 298195. PMID 2813372.
  28. ^ Ueno, H; Amano, S; Merecka, B; Kośmider, J (2009). "Difference in the odor concentrations measured by the triangle odor bag method and dynamic olfactometry" (PDF). Water Science & Technology. 59 (7): 1339–42. doi:10.2166/wst.2009.112. PMID 19380999. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 5, 2016. Retrieved June 4, 2014.
  29. ^ "Guidelines on Odour Pollution & Its Control" (PDF). Ministry of Environment & Forests, Govt. of India. May 2008. Archived from the original (PDF) on April 9, 2009. Retrieved June 4, 2014.
  30. ^ Benzo, Maurizio; Mantovani, Alice; Pittarello, Alberto (2012). "Measurement of Odour Concentration of Immissions using a New Field Olfactometer and Markers' Chemical Analysis" (PDF). Chemical Engineering Transactions. 30: 103.
  31. ^ a b c Jiang, J; Coffey, P; Toohey, B (2006). "Improvement of odor intensity measurement using dynamic olfactometry". Journal of the Air & Waste Management Association (1995). 56 (5): 675–83. doi:10.1080/10473289.2006.10464474. PMID 16739805.
  32. ^ Spengler, p. 486
  33. ^ "F.i.d.o.l." OdorImpact. Retrieved February 1, 2021.
  34. ^ "Odour Assessment". MFE.govt.nz. Archived from the original on November 22, 2012. Retrieved December 30, 2012.
  35. ^ Oracle Education Foundation (August 25, 2010). "Your Sense of Smell – The Senses". ThinkQuest Library. Archived from the original on August 8, 2011. Retrieved November 30, 2010.
  36. ^ a b Auffarth, B. (2013). "Understanding smell – the olfactory stimulus problem". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 37 (8): 1667–79. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.009. PMID 23806440. S2CID 207090474.
  37. ^ "Flux Chamber Measurements: Defensible Analytical Data for Evaluating Human Health Risk". Ceschmidt.com. Archived from the original on February 8, 2013. Retrieved December 30, 2012.
  38. ^ UNSW wind tunnel dimensions. Odour.unsw.edu.au
  39. ^ Young, Christopher A. (2010). "What Smells?". Pollution Engineering. 42 (5).
  40. ^ Dalton, P (2002). "Odor, irritation and perception of health risk". International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health. 75 (5): 283–90. doi:10.1007/s00420-002-0312-x. PMID 11981666. S2CID 9073422.
  41. ^ a b c d Engen, Trygg (1991). Odor sensation and memory. New York: Praeger. ISBN 978-0-275-94111-6.
  42. ^ Madaras, Lynda; Madaras, Area; Sullivan, Simon (2007). What's Happening to My Body? Book for Boys (Revised ed.). HarperCollins. ISBN 978-1557047694. Retrieved December 30, 2012 – via Google Boeken.
  43. ^ Glindemann, Dietmar; Dietrich, Andrea; Staerk, Hans-Joachim; Kuschk, Peter (2006). "Communication The Two Odors of Iron when Touched or Pickled: (Skin) Carbonyl Compounds and Organophosphines". Angewandte Chemie International Edition. 45 (42): 7006–09. doi:10.1002/anie.200602100. PMID 17009284.
  44. ^ Science in 1 minute: How does an electronic nose work (mp4) (Web). Universitat Rovira i Virgili. November 21, 2016. Retrieved July 26, 2023.
  45. ^ Belgiorno, Vincenzo; Naddeo, Vincenzo; Zarra, Tiziano, eds. (2012). Odour Impact Assessment Handbook: Belgiorno/Odour. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. doi:10.1002/9781118481264. ISBN 978-1118481264.
  46. ^ Zarra, T.; Naddeo, V.; Belgiorno, V.; Higuchi, T.; Dutta, N.; Bhuyan, M. (2012), "Instruments and Methods for Odour Sampling and Measurement", Odour Impact Assessment Handbook, John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 31–83, doi:10.1002/9781118481264.ch3, ISBN 9781118481264
  47. ^ Suslick, B. A.; Feng, L.; Suslick, K. S. (2010). "Discrimination of Complex Mixtures by a Colorimetric Sensor Array: Coffee Aromas". Anal. Chem. 82 (5): 2067–73. doi:10.1021/ac902823w. PMC 2947826. PMID 20143838.
  48. ^ Feng, L.; Musto, C.J.; Suslick, K. S. (2010). "A Simple and Highly Sensitive Colorimetric Detection Method for Gaseous Formaldehyde". J. Am. Chem. Soc. 132 (12): 4046–47. doi:10.1021/ja910366p. PMC 2854577. PMID 20218682.
  49. ^ Zald, David H.; Pardo, J. V. (1997). "Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: Amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation". PNAS. 94 (8): 4119–24. Bibcode:1997PNAS...94.4119Z. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.8.4119. PMC 20578. PMID 9108115.
  50. ^ Wrzesniewski, Amy; McCauley, Clark; Rozin, Paul (1999). "Odor and Affect: Individual Differences in the Impact of Odor on Liking for Places, Things and People". Chem. Senses. 24 (6): 713–21. doi:10.1093/chemse/24.6.713. PMID 10587506.
  51. ^ Herz, Rachel S. (2004). "A Naturalistic Analysis of Autobiographical Memories Triggered by Olfactory Visual and Auditory Stimuli". Chem. Senses. 29 (3): 217–24. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh025. PMID 15047596.
  52. ^ Epple, Gisela; Herz, Rachel S. (1999). "Ambient odors associated to failure influence cognitive performance in children". Developmental Psychobiology. 35 (2): 103–07. doi:10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199909)35:2<103::aid-dev3>3.0.co;2-4. PMID 10461124.
  53. ^ Chen, D; Haviland-Jones, J. (2000). "Human olfactory communication of emotion" (PDF). Perceptual and Motor Skills. 91 (3 Pt 1): 771–81. doi:10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.771. PMID 11153847. S2CID 1086223. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 4, 2016. Retrieved January 10, 2015.
  54. ^ Ferdenzi, Camille; Schaal, Benoist; Roberts, S. Craig (2010). "Family Scents: Developmental Changes in the Perception of Kin Body Odor?" (PDF). Journal of Chemical Ecology. 36 (8): 847–54. doi:10.1007/s10886-010-9827-x. PMID 20640943. S2CID 20084675.
  55. ^ Lenochová, Pavlína; Vohnoutová, Pavla; Roberts, S. Craig; Oberzaucher, Elisabeth; Grammer, Karl; Havlíček, Jan (March 28, 2012). "Psychology of Fragrance Use: Perception of Individual Odor and Perfume Blends Reveals a Mechanism for Idiosyncratic Effects on Fragrance Choice". PLOS ONE. 7 (3): e33810. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...733810L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033810. PMC 3314678. PMID 22470479.
  56. ^ Herz, Rachel S.; Inzlicht, Michael (2002). "Sex differences in response to physical and social factors involved in human mate selection: The importance of smell for women". Evolution and Human Behavior. 23 (5): 359–64. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(02)00095-8.
  57. ^ Berglund, H.; Lindstrom, P.; Savic, I. (2006). "Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 103 (21): 8269–74. Bibcode:2006PNAS..103.8269B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0600331103. PMC 1570103. PMID 16705035.
  58. ^ Wade, Nicholas (May 9, 2005) "Gay Men are found to have Different Scent of Attraction". NY Times
  59. ^ a b Larsson, M.; Willander, J. (2009). "Autobiographical odor memory". Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1170 (1): 318–23. Bibcode:2009NYASA1170..318L. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.656.6053. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03934.x. PMID 19686154. S2CID 40423777.
  60. ^ "Miller, Tabitha M.A. Smell". Tabithamiller.com. Archived from the original on December 31, 2012. Retrieved December 30, 2012.
  61. ^ Grammer, Karl (2005). "Human pheromones and sexual attraction" (PDF). European Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology and Reproductive Biology. 118 (2): 135–42. doi:10.1016/j.ejogrb.2004.08.010. PMID 15653193.
  62. ^ Weeden, Jason (2005). "Physical Attractiveness and Health in Western Societies: A Review". Psychological Bulletin. 131 (5): 635–53. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.635. PMID 16187849. S2CID 24782931.
  63. ^ Foster, Joshua (2008). "Beauty Is Mostly in the Eye of the Beholder: Olfactory Versus Visual Cues of Attractiveness". The Journal of Social Psychology. 148 (6): 765–74. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.616.6443. doi:10.3200/socp.148.6.765-774. PMID 19058662. S2CID 12985026.
  64. ^ Jacob, Suma; McClintock, Martha K. (February 1, 2000). "Psychological State and Mood Effects of Steroidal Chemosignals in Women and Men". Hormones and Behavior. 37 (1): 57–78. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1999.1559. PMID 10712859. S2CID 8218903.
  65. ^ Kohl, James (2001). "Human Pheromones: Integrating Neuroendocrinology and Ethology". Neuroendocrinology Letters. 22 (5): 309–21. PMID 11600881.
  66. ^ Grammer, Karl; Fink, Bernhard; Neave, Nick (February 2005). "Human pheromones and sexual attraction". European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology. 118 (2): 135–42. doi:10.1016/j.ejogrb.2004.08.010. PMID 15653193.
  67. ^ Penn, D. J.; Potts, W. K. (1999). "The evolution of mating preferences and major histocompatibility complex genes". The American Naturalist. 153 (2): 145–64. doi:10.1086/303166. JSTOR 10.1086/303166. PMID 29578757. S2CID 4398891.
  68. ^ Wedekind, C.; Penn, D. (2000). "MHC genes, body odours, and odour preferences". Nephrology Dialysis Transplantation. 15 (9): 1269–71. doi:10.1093/ndt/15.9.1269. PMID 10978373.
  69. ^ Potts, W. K; Manning, C. J.; Wakeland, E. K.; Hughes, A. L. (1994). "The role of infectious disease, inbreeding and mating preferences in maintaining MHC genetic diversity: an experimental test". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 346 (1317): 369–78. doi:10.1098/rstb.1994.0154. PMID 7708831.
  70. ^ Singh, P. B.; Herbert, J.; Roser, B.; Arnott, L.; Tucker, D. K.; Brown, R. E. (1990). "Rearing rats in a germ-free environment eliminates their odors of individuality". Journal of Chemical Ecology. 16 (5): 1667–82. doi:10.1007/bf01014099. PMID 24263836. S2CID 23968912.
  71. ^ Singer, A. G.; Beauchamp, G. K.; Yamazaki, K. (1997). "Volatile signals of the major histocompatibility complex in male mouse urine". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 94 (6): 2210–14. Bibcode:1997PNAS...94.2210S. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.6.2210. PMC 20066. PMID 9122173.
  72. ^ Dunbar, Robin Ian MacDonald; Barrett, Louise (2007). Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (1 ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 317. ISBN 9780198568308.
  73. ^ Wedekind, C.; Seebeck, T.; Bettens, F.; Paepke, A. J. (June 22, 1995). "MHC-Dependent Mate Preferences in Humans" (PDF). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 260 (1359): 245–49. Bibcode:1995RSPSB.260..245W. doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0087. PMID 7630893. S2CID 34971350.
  74. ^ Ober, Carole; Weitkamp, Lowell R.; Cox, Nancy; Dytch, Harvey; Kostyu, Donna; Elias, Sherman (September 1997). "HLA and Mate Choice in Humans". The American Journal of Human Genetics. 61 (3): 497–504. doi:10.1086/515511. PMC 1715964. PMID 9326314.
  75. ^ Thorne, Frances, Fink, Bernhard (2002). "Effects of putative male pheromones on female ratings of male attractiveness: influence of oral contraceptives and the menstrual cycle". Neuroendocrinology Letters. 23 (4): 291–97. PMID 12195229.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  76. ^ a b Thornhill, R.; Chapman, J. F.; Gangestad, S. W. (2013). "Women's preferences for men's scents associated with testosterone and cortisol levels: Pattens across the ovulatory cycle". Evolution and Human Behavior. 34 (3): 216–21. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.01.003.
  77. ^ Glidersleeve, K.; Haselton, M. G.; Fales, M. R. (2014). "Do women's mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1205–59. doi:10.1037/a0035438. PMID 24564172.
  78. ^ Havlicek, J.; Roberts, C. S; Flegr, J. (2005). "Women's preference for dominant male odour: effects of mistral cycle and relationship status". Biology Letters. 1 (3): 256–59. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0332. PMC 1617143. PMID 17148181.
  79. ^ Thornhill, R.; Gangastad, S. W.; Miller, R.; Scheyd, G.; McCollongh, J. K.; Franklin, M. (2003). "Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women". Behavioral Ecology. 14 (5): 668–78. doi:10.1093/beheco/arg043.
  80. ^ a b Gangestad, S. W.; Simpson, J. A.; Cousins, A. J.; Garver- Apgar, C. E.; Christensen, P. N. (2004). "Women's preferences for male behavioural displays change across the menstrual cycle". Psychological Science. 15 (3): 203–07. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.371.3266. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x. PMID 15016293. S2CID 9820539.
  81. ^ Garver- Aprgar, C. E.; Gangestad, S. W.; Thornhill, R. (2008). "Hormonal correlates of women's mid-cycle preference for the scent of symmetry". Evolution and Human Behavior. 29 (4): 223–32. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.007.
  82. ^ Thornhill, R.; Gangestad, S. W. (1999). "The scent of symmetry: A human sex pheromone that signals fitness?". Evolution and Human Behavior. 20 (3): 175–201. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(99)00005-7.
  83. ^ Rikowski, K. Grammer (1999). "Human body odour, symmetry and attractiveness". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 266 (1422): 869–74. doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0717. PMC 1689917. PMID 10380676.
  84. ^ Havlicek, J.; Roberts, C. S; Flegr, J. (2005). "Women's preference for dominant male odour: effects of mistral cycle and relationship status". Biology Letters. 1 (3): 256–59. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0332. PMC 1617143. PMID 17148181.
  85. ^ Alvergne, A.; Lummaa, V. (2010). "Does the contraceptive pill alter mate choice in humans?". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 25 (3): 171–79. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.08.003. PMID 19818527.
  86. ^ Roberts, C. S.; Gosling, L. M.; Carter, V.; Petrie, M. (2008). "MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives". Biological Sciences. 275 (1652): 2715–22. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0825. PMC 2605820. PMID 18700206.
  87. ^ Gangestad, S. W.; Thornhill, R. (1998). "Menstrual cycle variation in women's preferences for the scent of symmetrical men". Biological Sciences. 265 (1399): 927–33. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0380. PMC 1689051. PMID 9633114.
  88. ^ Kuukasjarvi, S.; Eriksson, P. C. J.; Koskela, E.; Mappes, T.; Nissinen, K.; Rantala, M. J. (2004). "Attractiveness of women's body odours over the menstrual cycle: the role of oral contraceptives and receiver sex". Behavioral Ecology. 15 (4): 579–84. doi:10.1093/beheco/arh050.

Further reading edit

  • Jarvis, Brooke (January 28, 2021). "What Can Covid-19 Teach Us About the Mysteries of Smell?". The New York Times. Retrieved January 31, 2021.